Imágenes de páginas
PDF
EPUB

paper sheet are slidden over each other, so that the left eye, its visual line, and its image of the coin c are all brought to the middle, while the right eye, its visual line, and its image of the pencil and of the point a are also brought to the middle from the other side, and superposed. We therefore see the image of the coin and trace its outline exactly an interocular space distant from its real position. If it were not for the screen, there would be another (right-eye) image of the coin and another (left-eye) image of the pencil and of the point a. These I have indicated in dotted outline.

Experiment 2.-If we make the experiment without the use of the median screen, then the cause of the phenomenon becomes obvious. If we lay a piece of money on a sheet of paper, and then gaze in the direction of the coin, but with the eyes parallel-i. e., on vacancy-the money of course separates into two images an interocular space apart. If we approach this with a pencil for the purpose of tracing the outline, we will see the pencil also doubled. If we now bring corresponding images in contact—i. e., right-eye image (left in position) of the pencil with the right-eye image (left in position) of the coin-we touch the coin with the pencil. But if, on the contrary, we bring the right-eye image (left in position) of the pencil to the left-eye image (right in position) of the coin, we may trace the outlines of the piece an interocular space distant from its true position. This is shown in Fig. 123, which gives the visual result of such an experiment—c and c' being the right- and left-eye images of the coin, and P and P' of the pencil. If, while the operation is going on, we observe carefully, we will see to the right the left-eye image of the pencil, P', engaged in making a tracing. But there is no tracing in this place; it is

only the left-eye image of the real tracing being made by the other pencil, P. In the previous experiment the screen cuts off all the images except the right-eye image

FIG. 123.

of the pencil and the left-eye image of the coin, which are brought together in the middle.

Tolerably good tracings of a picture may be made in this way. The only difficulty in making them really accurate is the unsteadiness of the optic axes, and therefore of the place of the image. I have, however, used this method in making outline tracings of microscopic objects, which may be filled out afterward. For this purpose a card is placed on the right side of the micro. scope, and the microscopic object is viewed with the left eye, while the right eye is used for guiding the pencil. Precisely as in the experiment with the coin (Fig. 123), the left-eye image of the object and the right-eye image of the pencil and of a certain spot on the card are brought together in the middle.

Experiment 3.-To trace the outlines of a light on an opaque screen. The same experiment may be modified in an interesting way thus: Set a light in front of you on a table. Place a median screen of cardboard or of tin between the eyes, so that the light can be seen with both eyes. Now bend the screen to the right so as to make a right angle at the distance of 6 or 8 inches from the eyes. This part will cut off the view of the

candle-flame from the right eye. Nevertheless, while gazing steadily at the flame, a really correct outline of it may be drawn on the opaque transverse screen, precisely as if it were transparent. This is illustrated and explained by the accompanying diagrams. Fig. 124 is the actual condition of things. ms, the median screen, resting on the transverse portion of the screen.

FIG. 124.

Fis the flame;

nose n; ts, the Now, just where

[blocks in formation]

the visual line of the right eye pierces the transverse screen, viz., at f, we may draw the picture of the flame F, precisely as if it were transparent. The explanation is found by examining the visual result, Fig. 125. By the heteronymous doubling of the median and transverse screens, the left-eye image of the flame and the right-eye image of the transverse screen ts are brought together, and the flame may be seen as it were

through the opaque screen as a transparency, and drawn at f'. In order to show that the flame is seen only by one eye, I have stopped one of the combined visual lines at the screen. The apparent transparency of an opaque screen in this case is precisely the same as the transparent borders of an opaque screen mentioned and explained on page 240.

FIG. 126.

Experiment 4.-To see through a book, a deal board, or the back of the hand, or even if necessary through a millstone. Roll up a thin pamphlet into a hard tube a half or three quarters of an inch in diameter, and hold it with the left hand between the thumb and hand, as shown in Fig. 126. Place the right eye to the end of the tube and look through the tube at the opposite wall, or still better at a map or picture hanging on the wall, while the back of the hand conceals the map or picture from the left eye. A circular spot on

the wall or map will be seen through the center of the hand (Fig. 126), precisely as if there were a circular hole in the hand. Of course a book or an opaque plate of any kind may be substituted for the hand in this experiment.

The explanation is as follows: The visual line of the right eye passes through the axis of the tube and pierces the center of the circular visible area of the object regarded, while the visual line of the left eye pierces the back of the hand or the book at a point distant from the axis of the tube just an interocular space, or about 2 inches. By the right and left shifting of the fields of view already explained, the two visual lines are brought together in the middle; and therefore the center of the area regarded by the right eye and the

spot on the hand or book pierced by the left visual line are also brought together and superposed.

One thing more to complete the explanation: The impression on the right eye prevails over that on the left the impression of the circular area obliterates that of the corresponding area on the hand or book for two reasons: first, because the circular area is strongly differentiated from the rest of the right-eye field of view (i. e., the dark interior of the tube), while the corresponding or coincident area of the left-eye field (the hand or book) is not thus differentiated; and second, because both eyes are focally adjusted for the distance of the object seen by the right eye only. Thus it happens that the right eye sees only the circular area, the rest of its field being very dark; while the left eye sees all its field except the spot corresponding to and covering the circular area. Thus the binocular observer sees the general field of the left eye (the hand or book), in the middle of which he also sees the circular area of the right-eye field. But if an ink-spot be made on the back of the hand or book just where the left visual line pierces it, the impression of this will be strong enough to resist obliteration; the strongly differentiated inkspot will be seen in the center of the circular area, as shown in Fig. 126.

« AnteriorContinuar »