Imágenes de páginas
PDF
EPUB

and therefore liable to error. Further, it is evident that the so-called facts of consciousness, in the one field as in the other, can not be considered reliable until subjected to rigid analysis. The study of visual (especially binocular visual) phenomena is peculiarly valuable: first, in teaching us that so-called immediate intuitions are in many cases only judgments, the processes of which have dropped out of consciousness; and, second, in teaching us the habit of analysis of such apparently simple intuitions.

RETROSPECT.

We have now given in clear outline the most important phenomena of vision and their explanation. It will not be amiss, before proceeding further, to look back over what we have passed, and justify its logical order.

There are three essentially different modes of regarding the eye, which must be combined in a complete account of this organ. We have taken up these successively. First, we treated of the eye as an optical instrument contrived to form a perfect image, every focal point of which shall correspond with a radiant point in the object. This is a purely physical investigation. Second, we treated of the structure of the retina, especially its bacillary layer, and showed how from this structure resulted the wonderful property of corresponding points retinal and spatial, and the exchange between these by impression and perceptive projection, and how the law of direction and all the phenomena of monocular vision flow out of this property. Third, we treated of the still more wonderful

correspondence of the two retina point for point, and therefore of their spatial representatives point for point; and considered how by ocular motion the two images of the same object are made to fall on corresponding points of the two retinæ, and their spatial representatives are thereby made to coincide and become one; and how, finally, all the phenomena of binocular vision flow from this property.

We have therefore apparently covered the ground originally laid out. But there are still a number of questions on binocular vision, somewhat more abstruse and more disputed than the preceding, but of so high interest that they must not be wholly neglected. The remaining chapters will be devoted to these.

PART III.

ON SOME DISPUTED POINTS IN

BINOCULAR VISION.

CHAPTER I.

LAWS OF OCULAR MOTION.

SECTION I-LAWS OF PARALLEL MOTION.-LISTING'S LAW.

WE have already (page 69) spoken of spectral images produced by strong impressions on the retina. It is evident that these, being the result of impressions branded upon the retina and remaining there for some time, must while they remain follow all the motions of the eye with the greatest exactness. They are specially adapted, therefore, for detecting motions of the eyes, such as slight torsions or rotations on the optic axes, which could not be detected in any other way.

Experiment 1.-Let the experimental room be darkened by closing the shutters, but allow light to enter through a vertical slit between the shutters of one window. Standing before the window with head erect, gaze steadily at the slit until a strong impression is branded in upon the vertical meridian of the retina. If we now turn about to the blank wall, we see a very

distinct colored vertical spectral image of the slit. Placing now the eyes in the primary position—i. e., with face perpendicular and eyes looking horizontally —if, without changing the position of the head, we turn the eyes to the right or left horizontally, the image remains vertical. Also if we turn the eyes upward or downward by elevating or depressing the visual plane, the image remains vertical. But if, with the visual plane elevated extremely, say 40°, we cause the eyes to travel to the right or left, say also 40°, or if we turn the eyes from their original primary position obliquely upward and to one side to the same point, the image is no longer vertical, but leans decidedly to the same side; i. e., in going to the right, the image leans to the right, thus

left, thus

[ocr errors]

; in going to the left, it leans to the

If, on the contrary, the visual plane

be depressed, then motion of the eyes to the right causes the image to lean to the left, thus- ; while motion

to the left causes it to lean to the right, thus

Experiment 2.-If, instead of a vertical, we use a horizontal slit in the window, and thus obtain a horizontal image and throw it on the wall as before, then, if the image has been made with the eyes in the primary position, it will be seen on the wall perfectly horizontal. Furthermore, if the eyes travel right and left in the primary visual plane, or upward and downward by elevating or depressing the visual plane, the image retains its perfect horizontality. But if, with the visual plane elevated, we cause the point of sight

[ocr errors]

to travel to the one side or the other, the image is seen to turn to the opposite side; i. e., when the eyes turn to the right, the image turns to the left, thuswhen they turn to the left, the image rotates to the right, thusIf the visual plane be depressed, then motion to the right causes the image to rotate to the right and motion to the left causes it to

rotate to the left (—).

These rotations of the image depend wholly on the oblique position of the eyes, and it makes no difference how that oblique position is reached-whether by motion along rectangular coördinates, as in the experiments, or by oblique motion from the primary position. Furthermore, the amount of rotation of the image increases with the amount of elevation or depression of the visual plane, and the amount of lateral motion of the eyes.

Experiment 3.-The fact of rotation or torsion of the images, and the direction of that torsion, are easily determined by the somewhat rough methods detailed above; but if we desire to measure the amount of torsion, the wall or other experimental plane must be covered with rectangular coördinates, vertical and horizontal. By experimenting in this way, I find that for extreme oblique positions the torsion of the vertical image on the vertical lines of the experimental plane is about 15°, but the torsion of the horizontal image on the horizontal lines is only about 5°. The reason of this difference will be explained farther on.

Putting now all these results together, the following diagram (Fig. 55) gives the position of the vertical and horizontal images when projected on a vertical plane for all positions of the point of sight. Simple inspection of the diagram is sufficient to show

« AnteriorContinuar »