Imágenes de páginas
PDF
EPUB

would recommend the following method: Let the two objects set up before the eyes in the first experiment be other than parts of the body of the observer—for cxample, a card and a rod, or two rods. Then, while looking at the table on which the objects lie, hold up the forefinger or better, a pencil-between the eyes and the objects. The pencil will of course be double. Now, by bringing the pencil nearer or carrying it farther, its double images will separate or close up. Bring the pencil into such a position that its double images shall exactly coincide with the centers of the two objects which you desire to combine. If you now look at the pencil, the ocular convergence will be exactly suitable for combining the objects.

In the cases thus far mentioned there is no illusion: the combined images do not produce the appearance of a single real object, as in the case of combined images of the same object producing single vision; because, in the first place, the two objects are dissimilar, and therefore the combination is not perfect; and, in the second place, the illusion is destroyed by the existence of the two other uncombined images. We next try -

2. Similar Objects.-If the two objects, the images of which we desire to combine, are exactly similar, then the combined image will be exactly like a natural object. For example :

Experiment 1.-Place two pieces of money of the same kind on the table, being careful that the stamped figures shall be the same and in the same position. Now, looking down upon them, combine as before. Not only will the outlines of the two pieces combine, but the stamped figures in the minutest details, so that the middle combined binocular image will have all the appearance of a real object. This is illustrated by

Figs. 44 and 45, in which the position of parts is reversed, because the eyes are supposed to be looking down. In Fig. 44 the two objects (coins), A and B,

[merged small][merged small][merged small][merged small][merged small][merged small][ocr errors][merged small][merged small]

are combined by crossing the eyes, and the combined or binocular opaque image will be seen at the point of sight as a'b, while homonymous monocular shadowy images, a and b', will be seen right and left. In Fig. 45 the combination is made by looking beyond the plane of the coins, and the coins in this case must not be more than an interocular space apart. The combined images, like a real opaque object, will be seen at the point of sight ab', and the two shadowy monocular images right and left, as before, only they are now heteronymous.

In this case, though the combination is perfect, yet the illusion is still not complete, because of the presence of the accompanying monocular images; but the formation of these may be prevented by the use of appropriate screens.

Experiment 2.--If in the first experiment with the money, before combining, we hold two cards, sc, sc', Fig. 46, one in either hand and at about half the distance to the table (the best distance is the plane of combination or plane of sight, for then there will be no doubling of the cards), in such position that the card in the right hand, sc, will hide the right piece A from the right eye but not from the left, and the card in the left hand, sc', will hide the left piece B from the left eye but not from the right, and then make the combination by crossing the eyes, the combined binocular opaque image will be formed as before; but the monocular images will not appear, because there will be no

[merged small][merged small][ocr errors][merged small][merged small][merged small]

other retinal image formed except on the central spots. This is represented in Fig. 46. In case we combine beyond the plane of the objects, then a median screen, sc, Fig. 47, extending from the root of the nose n to the table, midway between the objects, will prevent the formation of the monocular images, as shown.

Or the same results may be obtained without the use of screens by causing the two monocular images to fall on the blind spots.

Experiment 3.-Place two similar small coins on the table 5 or 6 inches apart and combine as before by convergence. Now looking steadily at the combined image, move the head nearer or farther away. At a certain distance the monocular images disappear and only the one combined image remains, looking like a real coin lying on the table.

But in these cases, although the union of the two images is perfect, and although we see nothing but an apparently solid opaque object, even yet the illusion is not absolute partly because the details of the table are doubled and therefore the table looks unreal, and partly because the eye is adjusted to the point of sight, whereas the light comes from the object, which is either nearer as in Fig. 47, or farther off as in Fig. 46, than that point.

This case may be varied in many ways. (a) Take a card and make in it two pin holes exactly an interocular space apart. Lay it on the face so that the two holes shall be before the two eyes. Each hole will be seen by its own eye alone, the other hole being hid by the nose. They will unite completely and only one hole will be seen in the middle, through which both eyes seem to look. (b) Spectacles when on the nose are a good illustration. There are two circles, one seen by each eye and hidden from the other eye by the nose. They therefore combine and we see but one circle in the middle through which we look with both eyes. (c) For those who are so fortunate as to have a friend with whom they can take such a liberty, we would recommend the following: Place the two faces

together, forehead to forehead, nose to nose, and gaze straight into the eyes. Only one eye will be seen in the middle. Those who are not so fortunate may make the same experiment on their own faces in a mirror.

We will

But in all the preceding cases, for one reason or another, the illusion still lacks completeness. therefore try still another case.

3. Many Similar Objects regularly arranged.—The illusion is most complete when we combine the images of many similar objects regularly arranged over the whole field of view, such as the regular figures of a tessellated pavement or oilcloth, or of a regularly figured carpet of small pattern, or of a papered wall of regular pattern, or the diamond-shaped spaces of a wire grating. In such a case, when by convergence we combine two contiguous figures immediately in front, other contiguous figures all over the plane also combine, and we see a phantom as perfect as reality itself, but with the figures smaller than the real. In other words, by the motion of the eyes in opposite directions in convergence, the images of the whole plane of the figured surface are slidden by one eye to the left and by the other eye to the right, until combination takes place again over the whole field. When the combination is effected, if we hold the point of sight steady, the combined images of the figures, at first a little blurred, become sharp and clear; and then the whole figured plane comes forward to the point of sight, and appears there as distinctly as a real object, but on a smaller scale in proportion to the less distance. This is represented in Fig. 48, in which the strong line PP represents the plane of the regular figures 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, etc. When contiguous figures, 6 and 7, are united by convergence at the point of sight, and seen there, then

« AnteriorContinuar »